NNNS: Appetite (2006)

Citation:
 
Study Design:
Class:
- Click here for explanation of classification scheme.
Quality Rating:
Research Purpose:
  • Does sweetness (with or without energy) contribute to overconsumption?
  • Does the replacement of a high-energy sweeetener (such as sucrose) with an artificial sweetener (such as saccharin or aspartame) lead to weight loss?
  • How do these issues relate to processes involved in weight maintenance?
Inclusion Criteria:
None reported.
Exclusion Criteria:
None reported.
Description of Study Protocol:
No discussion of how studies selected.
Data Collection Summary:
No comparison of standard variables.
Description of Actual Data Sample:
No comparison of actual studies by variables.
Summary of Results:

Appetite

  • Restrained by sweetness, carbohydrates or both?
    • The effect of caloric substitution (via an intense sweetener) upon appetite and the pattern of eating may depend on whether the substitution is made in a food or drink. The role of food products containing high-intensity sweeteners in the overall control of human appetite and body weight is still up for debate.

Early Studies

  • Rolls indicated that substituted carbohydrates do not offer any special advantage to the consumer for controlling appetite.

  • Anderson, 1995, provided evidence that ingestion of ≥50g of sugar within 20 to 60 minutes of a meal resulted in reduced mealtime food intake, which suggests that appetite regulatory centers respond to sugar's energy content. If sugars are replaced by a non-caloric sweetener, the regulatory centers will not respond in the absence of energy.

  • Naismith showed that an intervention with the substitution of artificial sweeteners for sugar caused an increase of 11% in total fat intake.

  • Lissner concluded that five studies on caloric dilution appear to indicate that reducing fat intake may be a more effective strategy for weight loss than consuming artificially sweetened foods and beverages.

  • Stellman et al, an epidemiological study, indicated that women who used artificial sweeteners tended to gain a little more weight over the course of one year than those who did not. The interpretation of these data is not easy and it should not be inferred that artificial sweeteners cause weight gain.

  • Colditz GA et al, 1990, a Nurses Health Study cohort, showed saccharin was the only dietary component significantly related to weight change in the same direction, both retrospectively and prospectively.

  • Kanders et al, 1990, showed females lost more weight when sweeteners were included, but male subjects lost less. Weight changes were not significant.

  • In Blackburn et al, 1993, during 19 weeks on a balanced-deficit diet, women were randomly assigned to either consume or abstain from aspartame-sweeetned foods or beverages. Participants were encouraged to continue with these dietary recommendations for the 2.5-year maintenance phase. Women in both treatment groups lost about 10% of body weight (9kg) during the 19-week treatment phase. However, during maintenance, the aspartame group regained less weight than the no-aspartame group.

  • Rogers et al, 1989 and De graaf et al, 1993, showed that under certain studies, sweet and non-sweet foods (equal in energy) do exert different effects on hunger and food intake.

  • Mattes et al, 1990, compared the effects of equicaloric breakfast sweetened with aspartame or sucrose with an unsweetened breakfast. Analysis of subsequent food intake (lunch, dinner, snacks) indicated a significant breakfast cereal x meal interaction. Post hoc tests indicated that lunch and dinner meals were significantly larger following the aspartame cereal than after either the plain or sucrose-sweetened cereal. This effect was greater in subjects who knew the composition of the breakfasts. Since under normal conditions consumers are usually aware of whether a product contains a low-energy sweetener substitute or not, such a response could well help to undermine self-imposed dietary control. The present data raise questions regarding the potential efficacy of aspartame as an aid in weight-management regimes.

  • Carbohydrate substitution by sweeteners?

    • Substitution of sweet carbohydrates with high-intensity sweeteners does induce caloric compensation by newer studies.

Author Conclusion:
  • The removal of carbohydrates from foods (by replacing them with a sweetening agent substitute) may weaken appetite control.
  • The use of a sweetening agent to improve acceptability of a high-fat food may serve to stimulate consumption.
  • The effect of sweetness on appetite control may vary according to the dietary preferences of different groups or individuals.
  • Sweetness has at least two functions in relation to the control of energy balance. 
    1. A role in changing the acceptability of foods and getting them into the mouth.
    2. A role in satiation and satiety by influencing the balance of carbohydrates and fat which is ingested, which in turn generates physiological signals to influence the mechanisms of appetite control.
Funding Source:
University/Hospital: University of Leeds
Reviewer Comments:
  • Gives questions and an overall conclusion with no strong outcomes with supported data
  • References are good.
Quality Criteria Checklist: Review Articles
Relevance Questions
  1. Will the answer if true, have a direct bearing on the health of patients? Yes
  2. Is the outcome or topic something that patients/clients/population groups would care about? Yes
  3. Is the problem addressed in the review one that is relevant to dietetics practice? Yes
  4. Will the information, if true, require a change in practice? Yes
 
Validity Questions
  1. Was the question for the review clearly focused and appropriate? No
  2. Was the search strategy used to locate relevant studies comprehensive? Were the databases searched and the search termsused described? No
  3. Were explicit methods used to select studies to include in the review? Were inclusion/exclusion criteria specified andappropriate? Wereselectionmethods unbiased? No
  4. Was there an appraisal of the quality and validity of studies included in the review? Were appraisal methodsspecified,appropriate, andreproducible? No
  5. Were specific treatments/interventions/exposures described? Were treatments similar enough to be combined? No
  6. Was the outcome of interest clearly indicated? Were other potential harms and benefits considered? No
  7. Were processes for data abstraction, synthesis, and analysis described? Were they applied consistently acrossstudies and groups? Was thereappropriate use of qualitative and/or quantitative synthesis? Was variation in findings among studies analyzed? Were heterogeneity issued considered? If data from studies were aggregated for meta-analysis, was the procedure described? No
  8. Are the results clearly presented in narrative and/or quantitative terms? If summary statistics are used, are levels ofsignificance and/or confidence intervals included? No
  9. Are conclusions supported by results with biases and limitations taken into consideration? Are limitations ofthe review identified anddiscussed? No
  10. Was bias due to the review's funding or sponsorship unlikely? Yes