NNNS: Weight (2006)

Citation:
 
Study Design:
Class:
- Click here for explanation of classification scheme.
Quality Rating:
Research Purpose:
Is there evidence that artificial sweeteners are useful both while dieting and at a later stage to prevent regaining weight? The influence of artificial sweeteners on aspects of physiology and eating-related behavior is therefore reviewed
Inclusion Criteria:
  • Medline (National Library of Medicine, Bethesda, MD, USA) was used to find relevant literature together with studies referred to in these papers.
  • Studies with one day for intervention studies.
Exclusion Criteria:
Description of Study Protocol:
Review of articles.
Data Collection Summary:
Description of Actual Data Sample:
Summary of Results:
Appetite

Some studies found that aspartame was associated with increased appetite.

  • Blundell, Hill, 1986: Glucose decreased appetite following its consumption. In contrast, aspartame increased the motivation to eat and decreased feelings of fullness. As ratings of hunger and the amount eaten do not always correspond, it is unfortunate that no food intake data were reported.
  • Rogers et al, 1988: The same group found that aspartame, saccharin and acesulfame-K all increased hunger, but they did not increase food intake after one hour
  • Tordoff, Alleva, 1990: Using chewing gums containing different concentrations of aspartame found that aspartame increased hunger.
  • Stellman, Garfinkel, 1986: Epidemiological evidence at about the same time found that the use of artificial sweeteners was associated with obesity.

Other studies found that aspartame may increase satiety.

Aspartame has been reported to increase satiety in some (Birch et al, 1989; Rogers et al, 1990; Hall et al, 2003) but not all such studies (Anderson et al. 1989; Leon et al. 1989; Rodin, 1990). A key difference may be that when aspartame has been found to induce satiety, it has been administered in a capsule, removing the sensation of sweetness.

The Renwick, 1993, review reported on research concerning three questions.

  • Whether artificial sweeteners increase perceived hunger: Subsequent studies failed to confirm the initial findings of Blundell, Hill, 1986. The addition of the sweetener to water without any other flavor may have produced an unnatural stimulus and atypical result.
  • Whether artificial sweeteners resulted in an increased intake of food at the next meal: Reviewed fifteen studies and found only one that reported an increased intake of food. He concluded that there was no evidence that artificial sweeteners increased energy intake.
  • Whether artificial sweeteners stimulated the release of insulin: Considered ten studies and found only one that had reported an influence on insulin release.

The Regulation of Food Intake

Short-term studies: It was reported that the use of artificial sweeteners results in a later increase in energy intake to compensate for the energy lost.

  • Birch et al, 1989, found that children under five years of age compensated completely for the energy removed from soft drinks by the use of artificial sweeteners.
  • Lavin et al, 1997, found that on days when sugar, rather than artificially-sweetened lemonade was drunk by young women, there was no difference in total energy intake, although the sugar-containing drink was associated with a lower daily intake of fat and protein.

Other studies have reported only partial energy compensation (Anderson et al, 1989; Rolls et al, 1990; Drewnowski et al, 1994). The extent of energy compensation may reflect the subjects.

  • Rolls et al, 1994, reported less-perfect energy compensation in young women who reported dietary restraint, while near-perfect compensation was reported in young men who were prepared to eat freely. Energy compensation is better in children than in adults.

Tables One and Two summarize studies that have examined the use of artificial sweeteners for more than one day. Early studies measured food intake by presenting it as a liquid pumped on demand into the mouth.

  • Campbell et al, 1971, reported that lean subjects adjusted the volume consumed when nutritive density was manipulated to maintain a near-constant energy intake. In contrast, subjects who were obese failed to adapt to changes in energy density.
  • Wooley, 1971, gave liquid diets to both lean subjects and subjects who were obese and found incomplete compensation when a lower-density food was supplied. The volume drunk increased when consuming a liquid of low energy, although the total energy intake was greater with the higher-density food.
  • Spiegel, 1973, found that when a dilute source of energy was introduced, 40% of subjects dramatically compensated by increasing their intake, although she found compensation was not usually complete and began typically two to five days after the start of the dilute diet.
  • Porikos et al, 1977, criticized these early studies as they used bland-tasting liquids that subjects who were obese did not consume in sufficient quantities to maintain weight.

Table One includes two studies that consider the addition of an additional source of sugar.

  • Raben et al, 2002, found that body weight increased when subjects were instructed to consume traditional soda containing 152g of sugar per day rather than the low-energy equivalent.
  • Tordoff, Alleva, 1990, had found that the instruction to consume additional sugar-sweetened soda increased body weight, although the use of artificial sweeteners prevented weight gain.
  • Vermut et al, 2003, reported that replacing (added) sugar by low-energy sweeteners might result in lower energy intake and reduced body weight.
Longer-Term Influence of Consuming Foods Containing Sugar or Artificial Sweeteners

Study Sample Treatment Duration Dietary Changes Weight
Liquid Diet
Wooley, 1971 Six obese and five lean males Liquid diet: Days One to Five, high energy;
Days Six to 10, low energy
15 days Incomplete compensation* for low energy
N/S
Campbell et al, 1971 Five lean males, two males and
four females who were obese
Days One to Seven, baseline; Days Eight to 30,
liquid diet
30 days Lean, but not obese, compensated
for low energy
Declined in obese with more dilute liquids

Spiegel, 1973

Two lean females and 13
males
Standard followed by dilute liquid diet 10 to 21 days Six of 15 subjects dramatically
compensated for dilution
Failure to compensate
resulted in weight loss
Porikos et al, 1977 Six females and two males who were obese
Days Four to Six and 10 to 12, 25% less
energy due to AS
15 days No compensation for lost energy Tended to increase with
sugar

Sugar Added to Diet

Tordoff, Alleva, 1990

21 males and nine females of normal weight

One to two litres of soda per day containing sugar or AS
Three weeks AS reduced energy intake

Sugar increased weight

Raben et al, 2002 35 females who were overweight and six males who were overweight Instructed to drink soda with 152g of sugar per day or AS 10 weeks Sugar drink: Fall in intake of fat and
protein
Sweetener: Fall in intake of sucrose
Sugar increased weight

AS=Artificial sweetener.
* The energy not consumed because of the use of AS was replaced with an increased intake of other nutrients. 

Table Two includes studies that have replaced sugar already in the diet

  • Poppitt, Prentice, 1996, suggested that individuals are better able to compensate for the energy dilution of a meal rather than energy supplementation.

Weight and Compensation for Energy Lost in Non-Nutritively Sweetened Foods and Beverages

Weight range has been studied; the energy saved by the use of artificial sweeteners has been included.

  • Gatenby et al, 1997, looked over an extended period at the effects of reducing sugar in the diet. The sucrose intake was found to decrease in those using artificial sweeteners, although overall intake did not differ from controls. A slight increase in protein intake was observed, although little overall effect on total energy intake or body weight was seen. Concluded that many reduced-sugar foods in practice are simply added to a pre-existing diet, rather than specifically used to replace sugar-containing foods. 
  • Bellisle, Perez, 1994, suggested that those who regularly use artificial sweeteners learn to compensate for the missing energy.
  • Louis-Sylvestre et al, 1989, gave low- and high-energy versions of an afternoon snack and measured the amount of food eaten during a meal one hour later. Initially the low-energy snack was not compensated for at a later meal. However, after consuming the snack for five days, precise energy adjustments were made. The authors questioned the value for weight control of food products with a lower than usual energy content.
  • Reid, Hammersley, 1998, found only that on Day One out of seven, did the use of drinks sweetened with artificial sweeteners result in a lower carbohydrate intake. The result supports those in the accepted weight range; the substituting of natural for artificial sweeteners does not result in a reduction in energy intake. There is consistent evidence of subsequent energy compensation. Although, on occasion, compensation occurred in the entire sample, in other studies, only some subjects responded. Attention should be directed to establishing the characteristics that determine whether energy compensation does or does not take place. When samples of the obese were considered, a different pattern emerged.
  • Kanders et al, 1988, found no significant differences in weight and appetite associated with the use of aspartame-containing products.
  • Porikos et al, 1977, found that substituting 25% of energy in normal food by using aspartame did not produce energy compensation. It may be critical that the energy-reduced diets were only consumed for three days, perhaps not sufficient time for energy compensation to occur.
  • Blackburn et al, 1977, reported that artificial sweeteners were helpful in a maintenance diet.
  • Poppitt, Prentice, 1996, reported that only when some available foods were high in energy did energy compensation tend to occur. When the entire diet was low in energy, compensation tended not to occur. They explained this difference in terms of energy density: If the entire diet was of low energy density and a constant weight of food was consumed, then there would be no opportunity to consume a food of higher energy density to make up the lost energy.
Longer-Term Influence of Replacing Sugar in the Existing Diet with Artificial Sweeteners

Study Sample Treatment Duration Dietary Changes Weight
Weight in Accepted Range
Porikos et al, 1982 Six normal-weight
males
Days One to Three, high sugar; Days
Six to 18, 25% less energy
24 days Days One to Three, none; Days Four to Six, 40% then
80% compensation* for lost energy
Initial gain prevented by fall
in energy intake
Foltin et al, 1988 Six normal-weight
males
Days Six to 11, 2,092kJ (500kcal)
fewer; Days 12 to 14,
original diet
14 days Days Six to 11, complete compensation;
Days 12 to 14, failed to compensate
Not reported
 
Louis-Sylvestre et al, 1989
17 males Snack: Normal or low-energy
(AS + fat)
Six days No initial compensation; Day Six, precise compensation
Not reported
Foltin et al, 1990 Six normal-weight
men
Lunch: 1,807 or 3,531kJ (432 or
844kcal) with or without fat and aspartame
13 days Complete compensation for low-energy intake No change
Naismith, Rhodes, 1995
Ten free-living males Covertly removed 2,092kJ
(500kcal) per day
10 days 85% compensation, drop in fat intake
No change
Gatenby et al, 1997
Forty-nine free-living
females
Replaced normal diet with low-sugar items
10 weeks Compensation, falls in sugar and
fat intake; energy intake remained the same
No change
Reid,
Hammersley, 1998
14 free-living males and 13 females
Replaced usual soda with
sugar or AS
Seven days Fall in carbohydrate intake
on Day One, then compensation
No change
Obese Subjects
 Kanders et al, 1988
59 obese
males and
females
Diet with or without aspartame 12 weeks Prescribed by study Weight, appetite N/S, with or
without aspartame
Evans, 1989
16 obese women who had slimmed Diet with or without aspartame Three weeks With or without aspartame N/S Not reported
Kanders et al, 1990
59 obese Maintenance: Behavior
modification and exercise
with or without aspartame for
one year
Prescribed by study Less weight regained with
aspartame consumption
only in males
 
Blackburn et al, 1997
163 obese females Diet with or without aspartame
Exercise with or without
aspartame
19 weeks; one to two years Prescribed by study
With or without aspartame, N/S
Less weight regained
with aspartame

 AS=Artificial sweetener
* The energy not consumed because of the use of AS was replaced with an increased intake of other nutrients.

Summary for Macronutrients and Obesity, Energy Density, Glycemic Index and Food Intake; High-Fat Diets

  1. Carbohydrates promote a feeling of fullness to a greater extent than fat, making it easier to overeat fatty foods. 
  2. The consumption of energy-dense foods is the best predictor of obesity. Energy density is almost entirely predicted by fat and water content. Given the importance of fat rather than carbohydrates, the use of artificial sweeteners by themselves is unlikely to have a major impact on body weight.
  3. Diets high in fat result in an increase in weight. Diets high in fat tend to be low in carbohydrates. 
  4. Reports of evidence that a low-GI meal enhances satiety are inconsistent. 
  5. Carbohydrate intake in general and sugar intake in particular are not the primary concern when considering obesity. In fact, a high-carbohydrate and low-fat intake is associated with a lower body weight.
Summary for Neural Control of Feeding
  1. Dopaminergic and opioid mechanisms in the ventral striatum mediate the response to palatable food. Artificial sweeteners and natural sugars have a similar impact on this area of the brain.
  2. The suggestion that blood glucose and insulin levels following carbohydrate consumption lead to 5-HT-induced satiety is not supported by the evidence. It has been argued that if the Wurtman hypothesis is valid, the failure of artificial sweeteners to increase blood glucose and insulin levels and hence stimulate 5-HT synthesis, could promote carbohydrate intake. Fernstrom, 1988, considered whether aspartame might disrupt the putative regulatory loop for carbohydrate intake and promote its intake. Aspartame increases phenylalanine and tyrosine levels in the blood, long-chain amino acids that compete with tryptophan for entry into the brain. The concern was that aspartame, via this mechanism, might decrease the levels of brain tryptophan and hence 5-HT synthesis, thus stimulating carbohydrate intake.
  3. Insulin plays an important role in the longer-term regulation of body weight, although it is unclear if such mechanisms are influenced by the lower levels of insulin that result from artificial sweetener consumption.
Satiety

Summary:

  1. The level of protein, fiber and water in foods predicts satiety. Fat levels are negatively associated with satiety, whereas the levels of sugar and total carbohydrates do not predict satiety.
  2. Energy-dense foods are not very satiating, but are highly palatable.
  3. The fat content of a food, but not the sugar or total carbohydrate content, predicts how much you want to eat.
  4. Artificial sweeteners do not increase energy intake or ratings of hunger.
Surveys of the Use of Artificial Sweeteners
  • Anderson, Leiter, 1996, related the consumption of sugar and artificial sweeteners to obesity.
  • Chen, Parham, 1991, showed that in female college students, the use of artificial sweeteners
    was associated with the consumption of less sugar; but in men, it was associated with a greater sugar intake. Overall, the use of artificial sweeteners was not associated with significant differences in the intake of carbohydrates, protein, fat or total energy.
  • Chen, Parham, 1991, concluded that the pattern of intake indicated that users expressed their weight concerns by choosing to use a high-intensity sweetener rather than by restricting their overall food intake.
  • Parham, Parham, 1980, showed the pattern differed from a similar earlier study where, in women, the use of artificial sweeteners had been associated with a lower intake of energy, protein, total carbohydrates and simple sugars. Gender and dietary restraint may be important factors.
  • Chen, Parham, 1991, commented that in some individuals, "the use of high-intensity sweeteners may contribute to an excessive sugar intake simply because users are more blasé about their intake of sweets."
  • Bellisle et al, 2001, examined a cross-section of the French population and considered those consuming artificial sweeteners. Those using low-sugar products were heavier, had higher triacylglycerols and poorer glucose tolerance.
  • de Castro, 1993, considered seven-day food diaries and commented: "Diet soda users ingest significantly less carbohydrate and have lower intakes than non-users. Individuals who used diet soda did not differ in their total caloric intakes on days or in meals during which they drank the non-caloric beverages in comparison to days or in meals during which they did not."
  • Tuorila et al, 1990, reported that those using diet soda are generally more concerned with their weight.

Non-Nutritively Sweetened Beverage Use

  • Levy, Heaton, 1993, found a common approach among those trying to lose weight is using soft drinks sweetened artificially

  • Mattes, 1996, was a meta-analysis of 42 studies that concluded there was little compensation for energy consumed as a liquid.

  • Reid, Hammersley, 1998, showed the energy compensation that occurred was replaced by existing soda rather than adding it to the existing diet.

  • Raben et al, 2002, considered the effect of artificial sweeteners in a sample of obese individuals who were feeding ad libitum. For 10 weeks they consumed either naturally or artificially sweetened drinks and food. The body weight increased in those consuming naturally sweetened, and decreased in those drinking artificially sweetened drinks.

  • James et al, 2004, an English study randomly gave school classes a one-hour session per term on the adverse effects of carbonated drinks. Over one year, those receiving the information drank fewer carbonated drinks, whereas the controls drank a similar number. The proportion of overweight children decreased in those attending the information sessions. There was a decrease in obesity when the consumption of energy-containing and artificially sweetened drinks were added together.

Author Conclusion:

In conclusion, obesity results from a multitude of factors, of which diet is only one. In turn, artificial sweeteners and sugars are only two of many aspects of diet. It is unreasonable to expect that modification of a single aspect of diet will have a major impact when isolated from the rest of the diet, basic physiology, behavior and the environment.

The data are so limited that definite conclusions would be premature. There has been no satisfactory study that has considered the value of artificial sweeteners in long-term weight maintenance.

Limitations

  • Subjects must be matched in future studies in terms of diet and exercise
  • Laboratory studies should be interpreted with caution, since short-term and the isolation of subjects from other factors that influence food intake may result in findings that cannot be generalized.
  • Volunteers often well-educated and highly motivated.
Funding Source:
Industry:
Sugar Bureau
Commodity Group:
University/Hospital: University of Wales (UK)
Reviewer Comments:
Quality Criteria Checklist: Review Articles
Relevance Questions
  1. Will the answer if true, have a direct bearing on the health of patients? Yes
  1. Will the answer if true, have a direct bearing on the health of patients? Yes
  2. Is the outcome or topic something that patients/clients/population groups would care about? Yes
  2. Is the outcome or topic something that patients/clients/population groups would care about? Yes
  3. Is the problem addressed in the review one that is relevant to dietetics practice? Yes
  3. Is the problem addressed in the review one that is relevant to dietetics practice? Yes
  4. Will the information, if true, require a change in practice? Yes
  4. Will the information, if true, require a change in practice? Yes
 
Validity Questions
  1. Was the question for the review clearly focused and appropriate? Yes
  1. Was the question for the review clearly focused and appropriate? Yes
  2. Was the search strategy used to locate relevant studies comprehensive? Were the databases searched and the search termsused described? No
  2. Was the search strategy used to locate relevant studies comprehensive? Were the databases searched and the search termsused described? No
  3. Were explicit methods used to select studies to include in the review? Were inclusion/exclusion criteria specified andappropriate? Wereselectionmethods unbiased? No
  3. Were explicit methods used to select studies to include in the review? Were inclusion/exclusion criteria specified andappropriate? Wereselectionmethods unbiased? No
  4. Was there an appraisal of the quality and validity of studies included in the review? Were appraisal methodsspecified,appropriate, andreproducible? No
  4. Was there an appraisal of the quality and validity of studies included in the review? Were appraisal methodsspecified,appropriate, andreproducible? No
  5. Were specific treatments/interventions/exposures described? Were treatments similar enough to be combined? Yes
  5. Were specific treatments/interventions/exposures described? Were treatments similar enough to be combined? Yes
  6. Was the outcome of interest clearly indicated? Were other potential harms and benefits considered? Yes
  6. Was the outcome of interest clearly indicated? Were other potential harms and benefits considered? Yes
  7. Were processes for data abstraction, synthesis, and analysis described? Were they applied consistently acrossstudies and groups? Was thereappropriate use of qualitative and/or quantitative synthesis? Was variation in findings among studies analyzed? Were heterogeneity issued considered? If data from studies were aggregated for meta-analysis, was the procedure described? No
  7. Were processes for data abstraction, synthesis, and analysis described? Were they applied consistently acrossstudies and groups? Was thereappropriate use of qualitative and/or quantitative synthesis? Was variation in findings among studies analyzed? Were heterogeneity issued considered? If data from studies were aggregated for meta-analysis, was the procedure described? No
  8. Are the results clearly presented in narrative and/or quantitative terms? If summary statistics are used, are levels ofsignificance and/or confidence intervals included? Yes
  8. Are the results clearly presented in narrative and/or quantitative terms? If summary statistics are used, are levels ofsignificance and/or confidence intervals included? Yes
  9. Are conclusions supported by results with biases and limitations taken into consideration? Are limitations ofthe review identified anddiscussed? Yes
  9. Are conclusions supported by results with biases and limitations taken into consideration? Are limitations ofthe review identified anddiscussed? Yes
  10. Was bias due to the review's funding or sponsorship unlikely? Yes
  10. Was bias due to the review's funding or sponsorship unlikely? Yes